• 开题报告
  • 社会实践报告
  • 申请报告
  • 研究报告
  • 党政报告
  • 可行性报告
  • 情况报告
  • 事迹材料
  • 申报材料
  • 述廉报告
  • 调查报告
  • 实验报告
  • 整改措施
  • 整改报告
  • 整改方案
  • 考察报告
  • 结题报告
  • 竞聘报告
  • 请示报告
  • 社会调查报告
  • 自查报告
  • 报告写作指导
  • 学习报告
  • 当前位置: 天一资源网 > Helping 正文

    Helping,ESL,Students,Become,Better,Readers:,Schema,Theory,Applications,and,Limitations

    时间:2020-02-19 07:49:32 来源:天一资源网 本文已影响 天一资源网手机站

    Nigel Stott
    nrstott@teacher.email.ne.jp
    (Fukuoka, Japan)

    Schema theory describes the process by which readers combine their own background knowledge with the information in a text to comprehend that text. All readers carry different schemata (background information) and these are also often culture-specific. This is an important concept in ESL teaching, and prereading tasks are often designed to build or activate the learner"s schemata. This paper summarises some of the research into schema theory and its applications to ESL reading. The author also highlights some of the limitations of the use of the schema-theoretic approach and points out the importance both of developing the learner"s vocabulary and of encouraging extensive reading.

    Introduction

    Schema theory is based on the belief that "every act of comprehension involves one"s knowledge of the world as well" (Anderson et al. in Carrell and Eisterhold 1983:73). Thus, readers develop a coherent interpretation of text through the interactive process of "combining textual information with the information a reader brings to a text" (Widdowson in Grabe 1988:56). Readers" mental stores are termed "schemata" (after Bartlett in Cook 1997:86) and are divided (following Carrell 1983a) into two main types: "content schemata" (background knowledge of the world) and "formal schemata" (background knowledge of rhetorical structure). Theories on the contribution of schemata to the reading process are discussed in the next section.

    Schema-theoretic research highlights reader problems related to absent or alternate (often culture-specific) schemata, as well as non-activation of schemata, and even overuse of background knowledge. Carrell, Devine and Eskey (1988:4) claim that schema theory has provided numerous benefits to ESL teaching and, indeed, most current ESL textbooks attempt schema activation through prereading activities. However, there may be limits to the effectiveness of such activities and there may even have been some over-emphasis of the schema perspective and neglect of other areas (see Eskey 1988:93; McCarthy 1991:168). Consideration is given in the latter part of the paper to the limitations of schema-theoretic applications and to the importance of "extensive reading".

    Schemata and the Reading Process

    In the process of reading, "comprehension of a message entails drawing information from both the message and the internal schemata until sets are reconciled as a single schema or message" (Anderson et al. in Hudson 1982:187). It is also claimed that "the first part of a text activates a schema... which is either confirmed or disconfirmed by what follows" (Wallace 1992:33) but the process begins much earlier than this: "The environment sets up powerful expectations: we are already prepared for certain genres but not for others before we open a newspaper, a scholarly journal or the box containing some machine we have just bought." (Swales 1990:88)

    The reading process, therefore, involves identification of genre, formal structure and topic, all of which activate schemata and allow readers to comprehend the text (Swales 1990:89). In this, it is assumed that readers not only possess all the relevant schemata, but also that these schemata actually are activated. Where this is not the case, then some disruption of comprehension may occur. In fact, it is likely that "there will never be a total coincidence of schemas between writer and reader" (Wallace 1992:82) such that coherence is the property of individual readers. The following section describes some of these differences in interpretation.

    Schemata and Differences in Comprehension

    Differences between writer intention and reader comprehension is most obvious where readers have had different life experiences to the writer"s "model reader". Readers sometimes also feel that they comprehend a text, but have a different interpretation to the author (see Hudson 1982:187). Humour is particularly vulnerable to misinterpretation as was discovered when a text entitled "It"s a mugger"s game in Manhattan" (Greenall and Swan 1986:197-8) was given to advanced L2 readers (Japanese). Although the text appeared humorous to the native-speaker teacher, it was found "scary" and "shocking" by the Japanese students.

    As Carrell and Eisterhold (1983:80) point out, "one of the most obvious reasons why a particular content schema may fail to exist for a reader is that the schema is culturally specific and is not part of a particular reader"s cultural background." It is thought that readers" cultures can affect everything from the way readers view reading itself, the content and formal schemata they hold, right down to their understanding of individual concepts. Some key concepts may be absent in the schemata of some non-native readers (such as "lottery" in Carrell and Eisterhold 1983:87) or they may carry alternate interpretations. The concept of "full moon", for instance, in Europe is linked to schemata that include horror stories and madness, whereas in Japan it activates schemata for beauty and moon-viewing parties (for ordinary people not werewolves!). Some alternates may be attitudinal: "gun" activates both shared schemata on the nature of guns and culturally distinct attitudinal attachments to those schemata (Wallace 1992:35-6).

    For learners reading at the limits of their linguistic abilities, "if the topic... is outside of their experience or base of knowledge, they are adrift on an unknown sea" (Aebersold and Field 1997:41). When faced with such unfamiliar topics, some students may overcompensate for absent schemata by reading in a slow, text-bound manner; other students may overcompensate by wild guessing (Carrell 1988a:101). Both strategies inevitably result in comprehension difficulties. Research by Johnson (in Carrell and Eisterhold 1983:80) suggested that a text on a familiar topic is better recalled than a similar text on an unf

    [1] [2] [3] [4] 下一页

    相关关键词: Helping

    • 范文大全
    • 教案下载
    • 优秀作文
    • 励志
    • 课件
    • 散文
    • 名人名言